PDF download.
Slime Molds – Social Amoebae
In this document I make the case for keeping slime molds in the future
Biolab in Calafou, and outline my perspective on them.
Slime molds raised to attention in the last years, mainly since the turn
of the century, and went viral with videos such as
this and
this. They were even
featured in XKCD, the acme
of geek fame. They have interesting properties that warrant scientific
attention and they are simple enough to study without an expensive
laboratory.
Why slime molds are awesome?
- They are the most intelligent brainless creatures!
- They can be used to build computers – OK, at least logic gates. :)
- They calculate shortest routes and build networks.
- They are one of the first organisms which lived on land.
- They are not animals, plants or mushrooms.
- They sport funny shapes and colours.
What can be done (in Calafou) with slime molds?
- We can keep them safely in petri dishes, etc.
- We can build mazes, make stop motion photos and show to visitors.
- We can make software to recognise and track slime molds on images.
- We can reproduce experiments done in prestigious universities.
- We may make mazes with 3d printers for them algorithmically.
- We might develop neural networks which simulate their behaviour.
- We might develop software which builds mazes for specific problems.
- We might collaborate with the
HAROSA
research network to solve logistics problems with them.
Notably, according to
some
sources
they can be interesting for measuring metal toxicity and even for
biological reclamation of heavy metal contamination, especially zinc,
etc. This can be potentially relevant for dealing with the contamination
of the Anoia river. Even if we cannot save the river this way, it may be
possible to contribute to scientific research by making and documenting
some experiments.
How can we get some slime molds?
There are three ways to get slime molds:
- buying them in
kits,
- getting them from somebody who has them,
- or harvesting them from the forest.
The last option seems good but there seems to be a lack of documentation
on how to do it. The second option could work if we make some more
research online, find the right people and ask them. However, for a
start, the first option is the most straightforward. I can invest in
ordering a few different kits and we can see where to go on from there.
In addition to the slime molds themselves, the rest of the necessary
equipment is trivial. Most howtos suggest oat meal flakes to feed them
and petri dishes to keep them. They do have a complex life cycle, but at
the moment I am not aware of any difficulties with keeping them alive
and multiplying for an extended period of time.
My motivation and research programme
As it will quickly become apparent, my interest in slime molds is bound
up with my interest in the ideological and historical issues of
cybernetics, and it is largely theoretical.
Slime molds embody several large scale changes in society and technology
which I study from the perspective of a critical history of ideas. They
stand at the intersection where metaphors are operationalised and
translated from one realm of reality to another (an ontological shift):
- Networks: networked creatures – master metaphor for everything?
- Computers: biological computers – computation as phenomena?
- Societies: social amoebae – the social body?
Networks: How a simulation replaced reality
At the same time as computers really happened, i.e. when the von Neumann
Architecture – which defines a computer as the combination of a (a)
processing unit, (b) a storage device and (c) a memory between the two –
crystallised (von Neumann 1945), neural networks – an alternative
computing paradigm – were being developed by McCulloch and Pitts (1943),
and successfully implemented by (1958). Even the founding father of
cybernetics, the mathematician Norbert Wiener, developed a similar model
during the same period, in cooperation with the Mexican physician and
physiologist Arturo Rosenblueth (Wiener and Rosenblueth 1946). These
people were all adepts of cybernetics, an ambitious research program
which originally aimed at building a functional model of the brain.
(Pickering 2010) Cyberneticians abstracted away the biological qualities
of living organisms (especially the brain) in exchange for a
mechanistic, calculative model – an approach that quickly turned into an
avant-garde scientific paradigm, redefining in those logico-mechanistic
terms such categories as life, purpose, reason and subjectivity. (Dupuy
2000) Therefore, from base research in the hard sciences, in a few
decades it became an ontological-metaphysical project, the effective
deployment of an ideology through the whole territory of social life.
(Tiqqun 2012)
To summarise, the key movement was comprised of two parts: first, the
abstraction of biological phenomena into a logico-mechanistic model; and
second, its reification from model to the very blueprint of reality. The
idea of networks in particular was drawn from the image of the
interconnected neurons in the brain, which was turned into an abstract
logical model, and finally reified to nothing less than an actual law of
nature. The idea of the network is thus interesting for its intellectual
trajectory from an observed biological phenomena through a scientific
model which aimed at understanding it to a concrete metaphor treated as
the nature of almost everything: a veritable ideal.
A principal example of how cybernetics shaped the intellectual history
of the second part of the twentieth century is Actor Network Theory, a
sociological research program developed by Bruno Latour primarily in the
1990s. (Latour 1993; Latour 1996; Latour 2005) At the moment the
hegemonic theoretical framework in the sociology of technology, it
presents itself as a “practical metaphysics” (Latour 2005, 50f),
granting equal attention and equal powers to both human and non-human
entities. The network of actors is the principal metaphor of its
sociological imagination. While it seeks to give an impartial account of
how networks are formed, function or fail, its ontological operation
restricts everything that exists – and can exist – in reality to these
same networks. If analytically actors are considered black boxes,
ultimately they too can be decomposed into networks. Thus nothing else
can exist in the world but networks – everything else proves to be an
illusion.
Of course the power of networks does not stop at the level of
intellectual reflection. We do not simply understand the world this or
that way – we also act based on such and such an understanding. When
everything can be seen as a network of networks, everything has to be
reorganised to become a network of networks. Communication
infrastructures, computer architectures, the global firms of capital,
its markets and the geopolitical strategies of imperialist nation states
– in conjunction with the very social movements which oppose them.
Humans start to live in the context of social networks and networking
becomes the principal professional activity, while liberal capitalism is
rebuilt according to The Wealth of Networks (Benkler 2006) (a book
playing on the title of Adam Smith’s The Wealth of Nations). In Manuel
Castells’ The Network Society, nothing else is allowed to live but
networks. When all problems are posed in the categories of network
ontology, all solutions are posed as network ontologies. Being a network
becomes the ultimate recipe for success – since everything else is just
a badly functioning network anyway.
The moral of the story is that cyberneticians started working on a model
which would correspond to reality more than the models before, and ended
up with a reality which has to correspond to its own models more than
before.
Obviously, slime mold research touches upon many of the issues outlined
above. Slime molds are living biological organisms which (are made to)
look and act like a network, and in turn used to model other networks in
the real world, with the idea of eventually generating a system which
can compute these networks on a logical level. The foundational notion
of slime mold research is that there is no ontological difference
between biological networks and logical networks, or any other networks
such as transportation infrastructures like railroads, the interaction
of the neurons in the brain, the collective behaviour of certain animal
populations, etc.
“In the province of connected minds, what the network believes to be
true, either is true or becomes true within certain limits to be found
experientially and experimentally.” – John Lilly, The Human
Biocomputer (1974)
Computing: Happens in the Brain, in Nature, and in Machines
As Dupuy (2000) explains, computers were not the material inspiration
for the cybernetic conception of the brain. In fact the cybernetic
conception of the brain was formulated before, and computers came to be
the material expression of it. However, in the history of cybernetics
there were many other research directions open. Thanks to the
organisation of cybernetics as a general science, and the involvement of
a high number of physicians in the movement (on both sides of the
Atlantic), especially biology, logics and computing were highly related.
Let me recount a few examples. Stafford Beer was one of the three
fathers of cybernetics in the United Kingdom (along with Ross Ashby and
Grey Walter). His life trajectory – from the chief consultant to the
British Steel Industry, through the architect of the Cybersyn project to
reorganise the economy of Allende’s Chile to yoga guru – could itself
fill a novel. His first forays into cybernetics, however, had to do with
biological computers. In fact not even biological computers. He was
firmly against computers. He was saying that it is really stupid and
selfish to build sophisticated machines that can count. Such a mistake
derives from the hubris of humanity, that we go around thinking that we
are the only creatures that can think. But every living organism is a
complex ecosystem which balances its inputs and outputs according to
well defined requirements: the requirements of its environment.
Therefore, we just have to look around and find the ecosystem we need
for our calculations.
In line with his ideas, he proposed to replace the management of steel factories with… a pond. The pond would do the calculations necessary to run the factory better than the human management. The main difficulty in this endeavour was bridging the gap between humans and natural ecosystems: how to do input and output? His best idea for input was saturating the pond water with steel powder, and using magnetic fields to give instructions to the ecosystem. However, as much as natural ecosystems automatically tend towards equilibrium, most of them cannot strive against a high concentration of metals – so soon everything died in his pond. [*]
His second idea was a living tissue arranged in a film which was pierced
at intervals with electric wires. He noticed that once the wires are in
place, paths form connecting the wires. Beer concluded that it was
evidently an example of adaptation, and eventually communication between
the human and the organism. Soon he prophetised that if confronted by
noise, the organism will develop an ear. In order to test his theory at
some point he was holding out the poor creature of his living room
window, so that it would pick up the noise of the passing cars in the
street. This second idea was not more successful then the first one.
However, Beer had good reasons to concentrate on biological computing
and therefore he refused the help offered by Alan Turing several times.
Turing was building a pioneering mainframe computer at the time, and
thought that the calculations Beer had in mind could be run as
simulations on the new machine. Towards the end of his life, Beer
returned to the idea of the biological computer and wrote a book –
accompanied by photographs of Hans Blohm – admiring the computing
capacity of the Atlantic ocean. (Beer 1986)
On the other side of the Atlantic, a hotbed of cybernetics, – in fact
virtually the only serious institution explicitly devoted to cybernetics
– has been the Biological Computer Laboratory, founded in 1958 by Heinz
von Foerster. Many key cyberneticians were visiting scholars there. For
instance the aforementioned ideas of McCulloch and Pitts were worked out
in that milieu. (Müller 2007) Inspired by the Mexican physician Arturo
Rosenblueth (Rosenblueth, Wiener, and Bigelow 1943) and the Chilean
biologists Humberto Maturana and Francisco Varela (Maturana and Varela
1980 [1972]), they believed that certain problems cannot be solved by
mere calculating machines like computers. However, they held on to the
idea that thinking is a logical operation which can be implemented in
various media, be it biological material, mechanics or electronics. The
first step in the realisation of this thesis was created by the
engineering student Paul Weston. His contraption, the Numarete, could
recognise the number of objects (or rather, shadows) placed in front of
it. According to Müller, “The Numarete was a computer that was not built
according to the (reductionistic) von Neumann architecture, but rather
was in a sense ’oblique’ to this architecture: it was based on the
parallel operations of its modules.” It was a custom-built electronic
box operating according to the principles of the aforementioned neural
networks. The whole laboratory draw its inspiration from the idea that
it is possible to build machines with capabilities of living creatures,
and the way to do it is following the logical operation of observed
natural phenomena.
Interestingly, the current epicentre of slime mold research is a very
similar institution, the International Center of Unconventional
Computing in Bristol, operated by the University of the West of England,
where its mastermind Andrew Adamatzky is building the slime mould
computers (Adamatzky 2010). The academic study biological computers
became virtually extinct following the spectacular success of the von
Neumann architecture, and even the development of neural networks was
put on hold for more than a decade after the publication of a book by
the adherents of the rival school of symbolic artificial intelligence
(Minsky and Papert 1969). As a result, bionicians around the turn of the
millennium picked up the threads close to the point where the
cyberneticians left them off. What changed, however, is the scientific
climate which is not ideal for base research any more, so that novel
efforts are not couched in the same level of epistemic-contextual
reflection as before, but more narrowly focused on narrow practical
applications. While some of the avant-garde idealism which drive
cybernetics withered away, many of the dangerous assumptions behind such
work continue to linger on without a critical evaluation. Such critique
is only possible from a perspective where two disjunct lines of inquiry
meet: the history of ideas conducted with a hermeneutics of doubt, and
the sympathetic anthropological field work which appreciates the
complexities of contemporary scientific practice.
Societies: Social Laws from Natural Phenomena, and Back
Natural laws as observed in biological phenomena often inspired and even
underpinned political thought. Hobbes’ Leviathan as the philosophical
imagery of the society as a unified social body is perhaps the most well
established example. The discoveries of cybernetics have not been an
exception. A logical order which can be abstracted from the behaviour of
living organisms and which applies in a more – or less – metaphorical
way to the world of human social affairs is a recurring theme in the
history of ideas.
The idea of autopoiesis and the related concept of self-organisation and
autonomy, (Maturana and Varela 1980 [1972]) as well as the idea of
ecosystems that tend towards equilibrium through negative feedback,
developed by cyberneticians, have been absorbed by the older tradition
of anarchist collective organising, mainly as conceptual metaphors.
(Curtis 2011) Anarchist ideologies depended for long on a positive
anthropology which asserted that people are generally good, but their
positive natural tendencies are short-circuited by the social conditions
in an authoritarian society (Newman 2007 [2001]). The corollary is that
when authoritarianism is not enforced by social structures, people start
to act in a more positive way, described in the language of solidarity,
mutual cooperation, and so on. (Graeber 2004) Anarchists found support
for these propositions in the scientific results stated above. Moreover,
another branch of cybernetics have also inspired anarchist theory and
practice in a similar way, namely chaos theory and emergence. Chaos
theory provided support for the idea that the actions of a small
minority (or perhaps even an individual) can have far-ranging structural
effects on society as a whole. On the other hand, emergence supported
the claim that horizontal social order rises up naturally wherever
people are left to organise themselves in the absence of oppressive
authoritarian institutions like the state. Interestingly, these
scientific trajectories have been developed most convincingly in the
area of emergent evolutionary theories, which stated that evolutionary
chains tended towards complexity and exhibited signs of spontaneous
self-organisation – therefore refuting or at least complementing the
idea of natural selection as the engine of biological history. (Wolfram
2002) These results are evidently useful in countering vulgar
interpretations of socio-darwinism which take the “survival of the
fittest” as their slogan.
Slime moulds entered this discussion once it has been realised that
while they live their life mostly as single-cell organisms, when they
face difficult environmental conditions such as the lack of nutrients,
they flock and form a single organism, joining their cells into a single
body. Recent results dubbed some species “social amoebae” – claiming
that they actually form a society of the species comparable to an ant
farm or a beehive. Since these animals have long been the subject of
study inspiring social theories, such a line of inquiry opened up,
offering great possibilities for ideological manipulation and
misinterpretation. Interestingly, the first discoveries suggested that
perhaps in contrast with ants and bees, there is “competition” and
“cheating” between certain amoebae when the cells which meet to form a
body have different genetic identity and materials. In these articles,
the language usually applied to the analysis of society is transferred
and applied to the understanding of micro-organisms. Observe the
following sample from the abstract of a recent article on slime moulds:
Altruism and social cheating in the social amoeba Dictyostelium
discoideum
The social amoeba, Dictyostelium discoideum, is widely used as a simple
model organism for multicellular development, but its multicellular
fruiting stage is really a society. Most of the time, D. discoideum
lives as haploid, free-living, amoeboid cells that divide asexually.
When starved, 104–105 of these cells aggregate into a slug. The anterior
20% of the slug altruistically differentiates into a non-viable stalk,
supporting the remaining cells, most of which become viable spores. If
aggregating cells come from multiple clones, there should be selection
for clones to exploit other clones by contributing less than their
proportional share to the sterile stalk. Here we use microsatellite
markers to show that different clones collected from a field population
readily mix to form chimaeras. Half of the chimaeric mixtures show a
clear cheater and victim. Thus, unlike the clonal and highly
cooperative development of most multicellular organisms, the development
of D. discoideum is partly competitive, with conflicts of interests
among cells. These conflicts complicate the use of D. discoideum as a
model for some aspects of development, but they make it highly
attractive as a model system for social evolution. (Strassmann, Zhu,
and Queller 2000)
Methodology
While I am reading about theories and theorists, experiments and
scientists, and so on and so forth, I’d like to try out these drifts of
the imagination in which you get entangled when you engage concretely
and practically with such creatures, experiments and phenomena. Beyond
merely reading the documentation about how ideas developed, what about
trying to recreate the existential and epistemological conditions which
moved such developments? I believe that certain experiences have
transformative have transformative power, and that a milieu can only be
grasped properly through developing some actual contributions to it,
however modest they may be.
maxigas, 2013-08-28→2013-09-02, Budapest
Notes
[*] It is interesting that slime mold research even touches upon this difficulty, albeit slightly. Note the section above on the resistance of slime molds to high concentration of metals. Additionally, there are even aquatic slime molds. Maybe Stafford Beer could go on with his experiment if he used slime molds.
References
Adamatzky, Andrew. 2010. Physarum Machines: Computers from Slime
Mould. Toh Tuck Link: World Scientific.
Beer, Stafford. 1986. Pebbles to Computer: The Thread. Toronto: Oxford
University Press.
Benkler, Yochai. 2006. The Wealth of Networks: How Social Production
Transforms Markets and Freedom. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Curtis, Adam. 2011. “All Watched Over by Machines of Loving Grace.”
documentary series.
Dupuy, Jean-Pierre. 2000. The Mechanization of the Mind: On the Origins
of Cognitive Science. Princeton, NJ and Oxford: Princeton University
Press.
Graeber, David. 2004. Fragments of an Anarchist Anthropology. Chicago:
Prickly Paradigm Press.
Latour, Bruno. 1993. We Have Never Been Modern. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
———. 1996. ARAMIS of the Love of Technology. Cambridge, MA and London:
Harvard University Press.
———. 2005. Reassembling the Social. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lilly, John C. 1974. The Human Biocomputer. New York: Bantam Books.
Maturana, Humberto, and Francisco Varela. 1980 [1972]. Autopoiesis and
Cognition: The Realization of the Living. Dordrecht, London, Boston: D.
Reidel.
McCulloch, Warren, and Walter Pitts. 1943. “A Logical Calculus of Ideas
Immanent in Nervous Activity.” Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics 5
(4): 115–133.
Minsky, Marvin, and Seymour A. Papert. 1969. Perceptrons: An
Introduction to Computational Geometry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Müller, Albert. 2007. “A Brief History of the BCL: Heinz Von Foerster
and the Biological Computer Laboratory.” In An Unfinished Revolution?:
Heinz Von Foerster and the Biological Computer Laboratory (BCL),
1958–1976, ed. Albert Müller and Karl Müller. Vienna: Edition Echoraum.
von Neumann, John. 1945. First Draft of a Report on the EDVAC.
Philadelphia, PA: Report for the United States Army Ondnance Department
and the University of Pennsylvania.
Newman, Saul. 2007 [2001]. From Bakunin to Lacan: Anti-Authoritarianism
and the Dislocation of Power. Plymouth: Lexington Books.
Pickering, Andrew. 2010. The Cybernetic Brain: Sketches of Another
Future. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press.
Rosenblatt, Frank. 1958. “The Perceptron: A Probalistic Model For
Information Storage And Organization In The Brain.” Psychological
Review 65 (6): 386–408.
doi:10.1037/h0042519.
Rosenblueth, Arturo, Norbert Wiener, and Julian Bigelow. 1943.
“Behavior, Purpose and Teleology.” Philosophy of Science 10 (1):
18–24.
Strassmann, Joan E., Yong Zhu, and David C. Queller. 2000. “Altruism and
Social Cheating in the Social Amoeba Dictyostelium Discoideum.” Nature
(408) (December): 965–967.
http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v408/n6815/pdf/408965a0.pdf.
Tiqqun. 2012. The Cybernetic Hypothesis. The Anarchist Library.
http://theanarchistlibrary.org/library/tiqqun-the-cybernetic-hypothesis.
Wiener, Norbert, and Arturo Rosenblueth. 1946. “The Mathematical
Formulation of the Problem of Conduction of Impulses in a Network of
Connected Excitable Elements, Specifically in Cardiac Muscle.” Arch.
Inst. Cardiol. (16): 205.
Wolfram, Stephen. 2002. A New Kind of Science. Champaign, IL: Wolfram
Media.